Skip to Content
Biotechnology and health

This is how your brain makes your mind

Your mind is in fact an ongoing construction of your brain, your body, and the surrounding world. 

Computer enhanced 3D diffusion spectral imaging (DSI) scan of the bundles of white matter nerve fibres in the brain.
Science Photo Library

What is your mind? It’s a strange question, perhaps, but if pressed, you might describe it as the part of yourself that makes you who you are—your consciousness, dreams, emotions, and memories. Scientists believed for a long time that such aspects of the mind had specific brain locations, like a circuit for fear, a region for memory, and so on. 

But in recent years we’ve learned that the human brain is actually a master of deception, and your experiences and actions do not reveal its inner workings. Your mind is in fact an ongoing construction of your brain, your body, and the surrounding world. 

In every moment, as you see, think, feel, and navigate the world around you, your perception of these things is built from three ingredients. One is the signals we receive from the outside world, called sense data. Light waves enter your retinas to be experienced as blooming gardens and starry skies. Changes in pressure reach your cochlea and skin and become the voices and hugs of loved ones. Chemicals arrive in your nose and mouth and are transformed into sweetness and spice.

A second ingredient of your experience is sense data from events inside your body, like the blood rushing through your veins and arteries, your lungs expanding and contracting, and your stomach gurgling. Much of this symphony is silent and outside your awareness, thank goodness. If you could feel every inner tug and rumble directly, you’d never pay attention to anything outside your skin. 

Finally, a third ingredient is past experience. Without this, the sense data around and inside you would be meaningless noise. It would be like being bombarded by the sounds of a language that you don’t speak, so you can’t even tell where one word ends and the next begins. Your brain uses what you’ve seen, done, and learned in the past to explain sense data in the present, plan your next action, and predict what’s coming next. This all happens automatically and invisibly, faster than you can snap your fingers. 

These three ingredients might not be the whole story, and there may be other routes to create other kinds of minds—say, in a futuristic machine. But a human mind is constructed by a brain in constant conversation, moment by unique moment, with a body and the outside world.


When your brain remembers, it re-creates bits and pieces of the past and seamlessly combines them. We call this process “remembering,” but it’s really assembling. In fact, your brain may construct the same memory (or, more accurately, what you experience as the same memory) in different ways each time. I’m not speaking here of the conscious experience of remembering something, like recalling your best friend’s face or yesterday’s dinner. I’m speaking of the automatic, unconscious process of looking at an object or a word and instantly knowing what it is.

Every act of recognition is a construction. You don’t see with your eyes; you see with your brain. Likewise for all your other senses. Your brain compares the sense data coming in now with things you’ve sensed before in a similar situation where you had a similar goal. These comparisons incorporate all your senses at once, because your brain constructs all sensations at once and represents them as grand patterns of neural activity that enable you to experience and understand the world around you. 

Brains also have an amazing capacity to combine pieces of the past in novel ways. They don’t merely reinstate old content; they generate new content. For example, you can recognize things you’ve never encountered before, like a picture of a horse with feathery wings. You’ve probably never seen Pegasus in real life, but like the ancient Greeks, you can view a painting of Pegasus for the first time and instantly comprehend what it is, because—miraculously—your brain can assemble familiar ideas like “horse” and “bird” and “flight” into a coherent mental image.

Your brain can even impose on a familiar object new functions that are not part of the object’s physical nature. Look at the photograph in Figure 1. Computers today can use machine learning to easily classify this object as a feather. But that’s not what human brains do. If you find this object on the ground in the woods, then sure, it’s a feather. But to an author in the 18th century, it’s a pen. To a warrior of the Cheyenne tribe, it’s a symbol of honor. To a child pretending to be a secret agent, it’s a handy fake mustache. Your brain classifies objects not solely on the basis of their physical attributes, but also according to their function—how the object is used. It goes through this process every time you look at a slip of paper with a dead leader’s face on it and see currency that can be exchanged for material goods.

white feather
Figure 1. Human brains can categorize this object by how it will be used. Computers using machine learning will only see a feather.
GETTY

This incredible ability is called ad hoc category construction. In a flash, your brain employs past experience to construct a category such as “symbols of honor,” with that feather as a member. Category membership is based not on physical similarities but on functional ones—how you’d use the object in a specific situation. Such categories are called abstract. A computer cannot “recognize” a feather as a reward for bravery because that information isn’t in the feather. It’s an abstract category constructed in the perceiver’s brain.

Computers can’t do this. Not yet, anyway. They can assign objects to preexisting categories based on previous examples (a process called supervised machine learning), and they can cluster objects into new categories based on predefined features, usually physical ones (unsupervised machine learning). But machines don’t construct abstract categories like “facial hair for pretend spies” on the fly. And they certainly don’t do it many times per second to comprehend and act in a massively complex social world.


Just as your memory is a construction, so are your senses. Everything you see, hear, smell, taste, and feel is the result of some combination of stuff outside and inside your head. When you see a dandelion, for example, it has features like a long stem, yellow petals, and a soft, squishy texture. These features are reflected in the sense data streaming in. Other features are more abstract, like whether the dandelion is a flower to be placed in a bouquet or a weed to be ripped from the ground. 

Brains also have to decide which sense data is relevant and which is not, separating signal from noise. Economists and other scientists call this decision the problem of “value.”

Value itself is another abstract, constructed feature. It’s not intrinsic to the sense data emanating from the world, so it’s not detectable in the world. Value is a property of that information in relation to the state of the organism that’s doing the sensing—you. The importance of value is best seen in an ecological context. Suppose you are an animal roaming the forest and you see a blurry shape in the distance. Does it have value for you as food, or can you ignore it? Is it worth spending energy to pursue it? The answer depends partly on the state of your body: if you’re not hungry, the blurry shape has less value. It also depends on whether your brain predicts that the shape wants to eat you. 

Many humans don’t hunt for food on a regular basis, apart from browsing in markets. But the same process of estimating value applies to everything you do in life. Is the person approaching you friend or foe? Is that new movie worth seeing? Should you work an extra hour or go bar-hopping with your friends, or maybe just get some sleep? Each alternative is a plan for action, and each plan is itself an estimation of value. 

The same brain circuitry involved in estimating value also gives us our most basic sense of feeling, which you know as your mood and which scientists call affect. Affective feelings are simple: feeling pleasant, feeling unpleasant, feeling worked up, feeling calm. Affective feelings are not emotions. (Emotions are more complex category constructions.) Affect is just a quick summary of your brain’s beliefs about the metabolic state of your body, like a barometer reading of sorts. People trust their affect to indicate whether something is relevant to them or not—that is, whether the thing has value or not. For example, if you feel that this article is absolutely brilliant, or that the author is off her rocker, or even if you’ve spent the energy to read this far, then it has value for you.


Brains evolved to control bodies. Over evolutionary time, many animals evolved larger bodies with complex internal systems that needed coordination and control. A brain is sort of like a command center to integrate and coordinate those systems. It shuttles necessary resources like water, salt, glucose, and oxygen where and when they are needed. This regulation is called allostasis; it involves anticipating the body’s needs and attempting to meet them before they arise. If your brain does its job well, then through allostasis, the systems of your body get what they need most of the time.

To accomplish this critical metabolic balancing act, your brain maintains a model of your body in the world. The model includes conscious stuff, like what you see, think, and feel; actions you perform without thought, like walking; and unconscious stuff outside your awareness. For example, your brain models your body temperature. This model governs your awareness of being warm or cold, automatic acts like wandering into the shade, and unconscious processes like changing your blood flow and opening your pores. In every moment, your brain guesses (on the basis of past experience and sense data) what might happen next inside and outside your body, moves resources around, launches your actions, creates your sensations, and updates its model.

This model is your mind, and allostasis is at its core. Your brain did not evolve to think, feel, and see. It evolved to regulate your body. Your thoughts, feelings, senses, and other mental capacities are consequences of that regulation.

Since allostasis is fundamental to everything you do and sense, consider what would happen if you didn’t have a body. A brain born in a vat would have no bodily systems to regulate. It would have no bodily sensations to make sense of. It could not construct value or affect. A disembodied brain would therefore not have a mind. I’m not saying that a mind requires an actual flesh-and-blood body, but I am suggesting that it requires something like a body, full of systems to coordinate efficiently in an ever-changing world. Your body is part of your mind—not in some gauzy, metaphorical way, but in a very real brain-wiring way.

Your thoughts and dreams, your emotions, even your experience right now as you read these words, are consequences of a central mission to keep you alive, regulating your body by constructing ad hoc categories. Most likely, you don’t experience your mind in this way, but under the hood (inside the skull), that’s what is happening. 

Lisa Feldman Barrett is a professor of psychology at Northeastern University and the author of Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain and How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. Learn more at LisaFeldmanBarrett.com.

Deep Dive

Biotechnology and health

This baby with a head camera helped teach an AI how kids learn language

A neural network trained on the experiences of a single young child managed to learn one of the core components of language: how to match words to the objects they represent.

An AI-driven “factory of drugs” claims to have hit a big milestone

Insilico is part of a wave of companies betting on AI as the "next amazing revolution" in biology

How scientists traced a mysterious covid case back to six toilets

When wastewater surveillance turns into a hunt for a single infected individual, the ethics get tricky.

The next generation of mRNA vaccines is on its way

Adding a photocopier gene to mRNA vaccines could make them last longer and curb side effects.

Stay connected

Illustration by Rose Wong

Get the latest updates from
MIT Technology Review

Discover special offers, top stories, upcoming events, and more.

Thank you for submitting your email!

Explore more newsletters

It looks like something went wrong.

We’re having trouble saving your preferences. Try refreshing this page and updating them one more time. If you continue to get this message, reach out to us at customer-service@technologyreview.com with a list of newsletters you’d like to receive.