Skip to Content
77 Mass Ave

Smart sleepers

An MIT study links sleep to academic performance.
A woman sleeping by books and a laptop
A woman sleeping by books and a laptopGetty

Materials science and engineering professor Jeffrey Grossman had long suspected that students who exercise perform better academically. To test his theory, he gave Fitbits to 100 students in his 3.091 (Intro to Solid-State Chemistry) class, and enrolled about a quarter of them in an intense fitness class. The students agreed to wear the fitness trackers 24/7 and give Grossman access to a semester’s worth of their activity data.

Surprisingly, the data revealed no measurable differences in test performance between the two groups. (Grossman speculates that the intervals between the fitness and chemistry classes were too variable to show an effect.) But while Grossman was tracking the students’ activity, he also inadvertently collected data on their sleep—which Fitbits track by recording lack of activity—and found striking correlations. 

Analysis of the data revealed a nearly straight-line relationship between students’ average amount of sleep and their grades (ranging from As to Cs) on 11 quizzes, three midterms, and the final exam. While other factors could be at play—and there were plenty of exceptions—the average overall course grades of students who typically got 6.5 hours of sleep were 50% lower than those of students who usually logged just one more hour of sleep.

The extent of the correlation was surprising—and is a strong indication that sleep “really, really matters,” says Grossman.

“Of course, we knew already that more sleep would be beneficial to classroom performance,” he says. But unlike most previous sleep studies that relied on self-reported surveys, “in this study the benefits of sleep are correlated to performance in the context of a real-life college course, and driven by large amounts of objective data collection.”

Contrary to popular wisdom, the study showed no improvement in scores for those who got a good night’s sleep right before a big test. According to the data, “the night before doesn’t matter,” Grossman says. “It’s the sleep you get during the days when learning is happening that matter most.”

Students who got relatively consistent amounts of sleep each night did better than those with greater variations from night to night, even if they ended up with the same average amount. Staying up too late also takes a toll. The data showed that if you need seven hours of sleep, it’s critical to start that seven hours before a specific cutoff—typically 2 a.m. for MIT students—or your performance starts to decline. “When you go to bed matters,” Grossman says. “Quantity isn’t everything.”

Keep Reading

Most Popular

Large language models can do jaw-dropping things. But nobody knows exactly why.

And that's a problem. Figuring it out is one of the biggest scientific puzzles of our time and a crucial step towards controlling more powerful future models.

How scientists traced a mysterious covid case back to six toilets

When wastewater surveillance turns into a hunt for a single infected individual, the ethics get tricky.

The problem with plug-in hybrids? Their drivers.

Plug-in hybrids are often sold as a transition to EVs, but new data from Europe shows we’re still underestimating the emissions they produce.

It’s time to retire the term “user”

The proliferation of AI means we need a new word.

Stay connected

Illustration by Rose Wong

Get the latest updates from
MIT Technology Review

Discover special offers, top stories, upcoming events, and more.

Thank you for submitting your email!

Explore more newsletters

It looks like something went wrong.

We’re having trouble saving your preferences. Try refreshing this page and updating them one more time. If you continue to get this message, reach out to us at customer-service@technologyreview.com with a list of newsletters you’d like to receive.